Importent of self-esteem

0 43
Avatar for Tina.4real
4 years ago

Social background is noted to be strongly related to an

individual’s destination in the social structure (e.g. Breen

2004 ; Jonsson et al. 2009 ). However, although social

background accounts for a vital part of the social

stratification in the labour market, a substantial part remains

unexplained (Bowles, Gintis, and Osborne 2001). In fact,

there are other aspects during childhood that have an impact

on labour market achievements later in life. Much of the

previous literature focuses on the link between cognitive

skills during adolescence and adult outcomes (e.g. Murnane,

Willett, and Levy 1995 ; Duckworth et al. 2012), but some

studies also focus on the link between non-cognitive skills

(such as self-esteem, sociability and communication skills)

and labour market outcomes (e.g. Bowels and Gintis 2002;

Harris 1940 ). Most prior research find strong positive

associations between high self-esteem and success later in

life in aspects such as work, social relations and health (Orth

and Robins 2014 ). For instance, previous research indicates

that e.g. self-esteem (Waddell 2006 ) and self-efficacy

(Chowdry, Crawford, and Goodman 2011 ) during adolescence

are important for labour market outcomes in adult life.

Furthermore, non-cognitive skills have also been suggested

as important for understanding gender inequality in the

labour market (e.g. Braakmann 2009 ; Fortin 2008 ; Mueller

and Plug 2006 ). Compared to men, women tend to have

lower self-esteem (Feingold 1994 ; Kling et al. 1999 ), which

may have an impact on gender differences in labour market

outcomes. To increase our understanding of the unexplained

part of this stratification according to both gender and social

background, it is therefore important to investigate how

non-cognitive skills (here measured by self-esteem, both

global and domain specific) during childhood are related to

labour market outcomes for young men and women.

Given that society is stratified by gender where cultured

beliefs about gender affect men’s and women’s behaviours

(e.g. Correll 2001 ; Ridgeway 1997 ; Wood and Eagly 2012 ),

the importance of non-cognitive aspects may vary by

gender. For instance, stereotyped assumptions about male

and female characteristics are often put forward as being

important for gendered occupational choices, ultimately

resulting in a gender-segregated labour market both in the

horizontal and vertical levels (Wood and Eagly 2012 cf.

Charles and Grusky 2004 ). For instance, that skills in

mathematic tend to be considered as ‘male’ skills have been

put forward as one explanation to women’s

underrepresentation in STEM-field (including science,

technology, engineering and mathematics) (Correll 2001 ).

Accordingly, there could be both gender differences in the

distribution of non-cognitive skills, and/or the importance of

non-cognitive skills for labour market outcomes may vary by

gender. However, to our knowledge, few studies have

assessed gender differences in the associations between non-

cognitive skills in childhood and labour market success in

adulthood.

Furthermore, longitudinal studies including rich information

about both children and parents are rare in this field. In

particular, there is a lack of knowledge on how aspects such

as childhood self-esteem and self-perceptions are related to

labour market outcomes in adulthood (cf. Gutman and

Schoon 2013; Sjögren and Svaleryd 2011 ). The present paper

attempts to partially fill this void by investigating whether

associations between self-esteem in childhood and

occupational prestige outcomes in adulthood exist and if so,

what the magnitudes of these associations are for each

respective gender.

A prerequisite for fulfilling this purpose is access to extensive

longitudinal information on respondents’ living conditions

both in childhood and young adulthood as well as access to

information on their parents. By combining parental

information from the Swedish Level of Living Survey 2000

(LNU 2000), childhood information from the Child-LNU 2000

(aged 10–18 years), and the LNU 2010 extension of young

adults aged 20–28 years, we have rich longitudinal data that

are well-suited for this purpose. Thus, unlike most previous

studies, we are able to account for the children’s social

background and their educational performance (school

grades) – two factors of major importance for their social

destination – and to link self-esteem in childhood to later

labour market outcomes for a representative sample of

Swedish boys and girls. The relationship between self-esteem

and important life outcomes is debated in previous literature.

Thus, it is not clear-cut whether self-esteem is a cause or a

consequence of, for example, career success (e.g. Heckman,

Stixrud, and Urzua 2006; Orth, Robins, and Widamen 2012).

One of the strengths of the present study is that the

information on self-esteem is measured before the labour

market outcome.

2
$ 0.42
$ 0.42 from @Preshylite
Sponsors of Tina.4real
empty
empty
empty
Avatar for Tina.4real
4 years ago

Comments